Skip to Main Content
San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance logo
San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance Library logo

Giant Panda (Ailuropoda melanoleuca) Fact Sheet: Behavior & Ecology

Update in Progress

Dear Readers,

Some of the information in this fact sheet, like a panda, has become fuzzy. San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance is working to bring you an updated version of the Giant Panda Fact Sheet with additional science and conservation information. Thanks for your patience, as we quietly leaf through the research literature.

Please check back soon. SDZWA team members can email questions to library@sdzwa.org.

Want more panda facts to munch on? Read SDZWA's latest Stories and news releases.

Activity Cycle

Active day and night

  • General activity
    • Inactive for long period
      • May spend 41% of the day at rest, c. 9.8 hours
      • Most rest periods last 2-4 hours
    • Activity peaks at dawn and dusk (crepuscular)
      • Some activity occurs throughout all times of the day and night
      • Accounts for c. 59% of the daytime hours, c. 14.2 hours
      • Activity increases in June and November, though levels are also associated with variation in weather
  • Do not hibernate
    • Unlike other bears
    • Constant access to food makes dormant period unnecessary
    • Bamboo is a poor nutrient source; pandas can't eat enough to gain fat deposits needed for hibernation
  • Drink at least once/day
    • Consume water from puddles or streams
  • Behavior supports energy conservation
    • Avoid travel up steep slopes
    • Communicate with scent rather than undertaking direct confrontation
    • Do not defend territorial boundaries
    • Remain within in small ranges

Daily activity pattern

  • Activity in the wild, one study (Schaller et al. 1995)
    • Rest 41% of time
    • Feed 55% of time
    • Travel 2% of time
    • Other actions (scent-marking, grooming) 2% of time
  • Activity in managed care (Mainka & Zhang 1994)
    • Sleep 40% of time
    • Rest 21% of time
    • Feed 25% of time
    • Walk 13% of time
    • Socialize 1% of time

Home Range

Home range size

  • Males with larger ranges than females (Schaller et al. 1985; Yong et al. 2004; Zhang et al. 2014)
  • Extreme values 5-18 km2 (as reported in Garshelis 2004; Zhange et al. 2014)
    • 4-6.5 km2, one study, though authors recognize the value is likely an underestimate of the true range size (Schaller et al. 1985)
    • c. 11.5 km2, estimate based on newer technology, high-resolution GPS telemetry (Zhang et al. 2014)

Use of range

  • Inhabit only a fraction (10%) of range each month
  • Usage seasonally variable
    • Descending slowly to lower elevations for winter, studies in Foping Nature Reserve, China; transition time lasting c. 36 days (Yong et al. 2004)
    • Returning to summer ranges more quickly; <1 month, though observations of as little as 2-3 days reported (Yong et al. 2004; Zhang et al. 2014)
    • Degree of seasonality may be dependent on population location (Yong et al. 2004)
    • Winter home range size larger than in summer, though individual variability is considerable (Yong et al. 2004; Zhang et al. 2014)

Range overlap

  • Home ranges overlap, but direct encounter between pandas are rare (Wei et al. 2015)
  • Male ranges overlap extensively (Schaller et al. 1985)
    • One large male appears to dominate the use of the territory
    • Frequent chemical marking at "scent-stations" and travel routes help individuals avoid each other
    • Combat between males generally rare; though commonly observed at certain times, when females are in estrous (Nie et al. 2012)
  • Female range perimeters often overlap those of others, female and male (Schaller et al. 1985)
    • In practice, however, females stay in a core area that has no overlap with other females
      • Female/female competition may be driven by access to limited den sites (Schaller et al. 1985)
      • Surprisingly, GPS-collared female recently shown to move up to 50 km (31 mi) out of her home range during mating season (Swaisgood et al. 2009)

Fidelity to core habitat

  • Returning to same foraging areas suggests a well-developed spatial memory (Swaisgood et al. 2016)

Social Groups

Solitary most often

  • Female remains alone, unless caring for young
  • Breeding interactions brief and infrequent
    • Interactions last only a few days
    • Strong competition may occur between males in a breeding season

Polygynous mating system

  • A single males will mate with multiple females (Schaller et al. 2002)

Adapt reproductive behavior to soliary context

  • Exhibit considerable flexibility communication cues (e.g., signaling reproductive status) (Owen et al. 2016b)

Communication

Displays/visual signals

  • Striking coloration may send signal to other members of the species
  • Direct confrontation involves body posture and vocalization
    • Lowered head stare and head-bobbing may be accompanied by growl and/or roar
    • Ears do not move as in other species
  • Aggression
    • Swatting with forepaws
    • Lunging
    • Grappling
    • Biting
  • Lack of aggression
    • Turning head
    • Dropping head
    • Covering eyes and muzzle with paws

Vocalization

  • Vocalization is infrequent
    • Associated with courting, social interactions
    • Not similar to the roars and growls of other bears, in general (Loeffler et al. 2006)
      • Black ear and giant panda both moan to indicate slight aggression
      • A high-pitched chirp (yip) may be unique for giant pandas (Schaller et al. 1985)
  • Females utter distinctive chirps that advertise their fertile phase (Charlton et al 2009)
    • In an experimental setting, males are much more attracted by fertile phase chrips
  • Male pandas bleats provide information about their testosterone levels (Charlton et al 2010)
    • Male vocalizations were recorded and matched with the caller's hormone profile
    • Higher testosterone levels correlated with longer lasting bleats and changes in sound quality
    • Females can use this information to choose between males
    • Males can use these cues to avoid potentially dangerous rivals
  • 11 distinctive vocal and unvoiced (jaw-pop) sounds identified (Kleiman & Peters 1990)
    • Bleat is social/friendly; conveys information about caller identity (studies by Charlton et al.)
    • Huff, snort, chomp indicate apprehension
    • Honk, squeal indicate distress
    • Growl, roar indicates aggression
    • Moan, bark, yip, chirp may be emitted in varying combinations and with varying intensities
  • Giant pandas are the only bears that do not hum (Peters et al. 2007)

Olfactory signals

  • Sophisticated sense of smell (White et al. 2003) (Swaisgood et al. 2002) (Swaisgood et al. 2004)
    • Can detect sex, age, reproductive condition of scent maker
    • Able to distinguish identity of individuals
    • Can tell kin (related individuals) from nonkin using urine and body odor (Swaisgood et al. 2016)
      • Females interested in scents of unrelated females
    • May differentiate time since deposition of scent mark
  • Leave both anogenital gland and urination scent marks (from White et al. 2003)
    • Deposit scent on surfaces of trees and rocks
  • Mark environment at communal scent stations and along established travel routes
    • Bark stripping (by biting or clawing)
    • Ground pawing
    • Rubbing and rolling
    • Tree scenting with secretions from anogenital glands
    • Urination
  • Chemical communication in pandas in managed care reveal (Swaisgood et al. 2000) (White et al. 2002):
    • Four marking postures by pandas possible, at various heights:
      • Squat (deposits scent on ground) - most commonly used by females
      • Reverse (leaves scent by backing up to vertical surface) - most commonly used by adults
      • Legcock (leaves scent with one leg raised against vertical surface) - used more often by males
      • Handstand (scent and urine marks left high on vertical surface) - used by adult males (Swaisgood et al. 2000)
    • Individuals respond depending on height of odor mark
      • Highest marks caused more investigative behavior
      • High marks may indicate male's competitive abilities and aggressive intent
    • Females also convey competitive status by making high-placed scent marks
      • Females don't compete with each other over males or food resources, but perhaps over rare denning sites

Play

Participants

  • Primarily an activity of young

Forms of play

  • Tumble and roll
    • Partial and complete somersaults
    • Lateral rolls
    • Body twisting
  • Slide down snowy slopes

Locomotion

Gait similar to other bears

  • Extremely agile
  • Stride of pandas is typically longer
  • Walk with a rolling gait
    • Described as "ambling", "pigeon-toed", or "waddling"
      • Head held low
    • Limbs on the same side move forward and backward at the same time
  • Rarely gallop
    • Rapid movement through thick bamboo is difficult
  • Climb into trees
    • Move into trees to sun, rest, and escape danger
      • Individuals may move into trees to avoid sexual advances
    • Young climb more easily than adults
      • Offspring often remain in a tree while mother forages (Lü et al. 1994)

Travel

  • Speed of travel
    • Move c. 2-3 km/hr
  • Cover short distances each day, typically
    • May travel less than 1 km/day
  • Most movement related to foraging

Interspecies Interactions

Competitors

  • Mammalian "stem and leaf" eaters
    • Golden monkeys, takin, red pandas, Asiatic black bears, and many pheasants live in the same geographic areas as giant pandas
      • Red panda and bamboo rats also consume bamboo
      • Monkeys consume leaves
      • Ungulates unlikely to greatly impact panda numbers as they are few in number
        • Sambar deer, serow, and tufted deer live in the same geographic areas as giant pandas
    • Presence of some may impact the number of pandas an area can support

Predators

  • Juveniles and young most susceptible
    • Yellow-throated marten, weasel, golden cats, dholes prey on juvenile giant pandas

Panda at Rest

Giant Panda in a tree

Pandas are adept tree climbers when seeking a bit of sun, some peace and quiet, or safe refuge.

Image credit: © San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance. All rights reserved.

Page Citations

Catton (1990)
Charlton et al. (2009)
Charlton et al. (2010)
Garshelis (2004)
Hagey & MacDonald (2003)
Kleiman & Peters (1990)
Lü et al. (2008)
Mainka & Zhang (1994)
Nie et al. (2012)
Schaller et al. (1985)
Swaisgood et al. (2000)
White et al. (2002, 2003)
Zhang et al. (2014)

SDZWA Library Links