Skip to Main Content
San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance logo
San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance Library logo

Giant Panda (Ailuropoda melanoleuca) Fact Sheet: Behavior & Ecology

Update in Progress

Dear Readers,

Some of the information in this fact sheet, like a panda, has become fuzzy. San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance is working to bring you an updated version of the Giant Panda Fact Sheet with additional science and conservation information. Thanks for your patience, as we quietly leaf through the research literature.

In the meantime, this recent book chapter by Swaisgood et al. (2020) presents an outstanding summary of giant panda biology.

Please check back soon. SDZWA team members can email questions to library@sdzwa.org.

Want to munch on more panda facts? Read SDZWA's latest Stories and news releases.

Activity Cycle

Daily patterns

  • Time of day
    • Active day and night (Schaller et al. 1985)
      • Active for a few hours at a time (“bursts” of activity) (Garshelis 2009b)
  • Conserving energy
    • Spend much of the day resting/sleeping (to conserve energy) and also eating (see Schaller et al. 1985, Ch. 5)
      • Lots of bamboo required to meet their daily energy needs
        • See Diet & Feeding
      • Inactive for long periods
    • Active for about 50% of the day (Garshelis 2009b)
      • Most activity involves foraging and eating (Garshelis 2009b)
    • Least active when feeding on abundant or nutritious bamboo leaves (Garshelis 2009b)
      • Differs from other bears that are most active when food is abundant (Garshelis 2009b)
  • Season and weather also influence activity levels (Schaller et al. 1985)
  • Influence of reproduction
    • Mothers with young cubs much less active; spend considerable time at den site (Schaller et al. 1985)

Seasonal/annual patterns

  • Little variation in seasonal activity (Garshelis 2009b)
  • Do not hibernate, unlike other bears (Garshelis 2009b)
    • Food available year-round (Garshelis 2009b)
    • During winter, seek shelter from snow and cold conditions in rocky areas, caves, and hollow trees (Garshelis 2009b)

Home Range

Home ranges

  • Home range size
    • On average, about 5 to 15 km2 (Schaller et al. 1985; Garshelis 2009b)
      • Range: 1 to 60 km2 (Garshelis 2009b)
      • Influenced by season, habitat, social interactions, etc. (Garshelis 2009b)
    • Seasonal differences
      • Home ranges generally larger in winter than in summer (eg, Zhang et al. 2014)
        • Varies considerably among individuals
      • See “Seasonal movements,” below
  • Home range overlap and area use
    • Individuals have small, overlapping home ranges (Schaller et al. 1985; Garshelis 2009b)
      • However, core areas used by pandas of the same sex infrequently overlap (Garshelis 2009b), reducing direct contact among these individuals (Schaller et al. 1985)
      • Spacing maintained through scent marking (Garshelis 2009b)
        • See “Smell and scent marking”
    • Males spend time at preferred sites within their home range, but do not use core areas, as females do (Schaller et al. 1985)
      • Males roam more widely, using core areas used by “females and subadults” (Schaller et al. 1985)
    • Habitat use changes seasonally (Garshelis 2009b; Zhang et al. 2012)
      • See “Seasonal movements,” below
    • A male’s core range may overlap that of several females (Garshelis 2009b)
      • See “Mating system”

Daily movements

  • Typically move short distances each day (Schaller et al. 1985)
    • Foraging patches closely spaced (Schaller et al. 1985)
    • Schaller et al. (1985) found that pandas move less than 1 km per day (linear, straight-line distance)

Seasonal movements

  • General patterns
    • Have distinct winter and summer habitat areas (Garshelis 2009b; Zhang et al. 2014)
      • Shift elevation, seeking preferred seasonal temperatures (Garshelis 2009b and studies cited below)
      • Move with changing growth of various bamboo species (Garshelis 2009b)
  • Winter (and early spring)
    • Spend time at lower elevations to avoid harshest cold temperatures (Chorn and Hoffman 1978; Garshelis 2009b; Zhang et al. 2012; Zhang et al. 2014)
    • Descend slowly, taking several weeks to reach habitat areas lower elevations (Yong et al. 2004; Zhang et al. 2014)
  • Summer
    • Spend time at higher elevations, seeking cooler temperatures (Garshelis 2009b; Zhang et al. 2012)
      • Giant pandas are sensitive to heat

Dispersal

  • Unlike other bear species, females (instead of males) appear to disperse from their natal area (Garshelis 2009b)
    • Males stay nearer to mother’s home range (Garshelis 2009b)

Social Groups

Generally solitary

(Chorn and Hoffman 1978; Schaller et al. 1985)

  • Females and males typically live singly (Chorn and Hoffman 1978)
  • Females and males most commonly interact during breeding (Chorn and Hoffman 1978)
    • See Reproduction
  • Females live singly, unless caring for young (Chorn and Hoffman 1978)
    • Cubs accompany mother until reaching independence (Chorn and Hoffman 1978)
      • See Life Stages

Communication

Smell and scent marking

  • Leaving scents
    • Mark environment at communal scent stations and along established travel routes (Kleiman et al. 1979)
      • Secretions build up over time at frequented locations (Kleiman et al. 1979)
    • Leave marks on trees, rocks, logs, stumps, etc. using gland secretions and urine
    • Use a variety of body postures, including squatting, backing up to a surface, raising a hindleg, and among males, a “headstand” position (Morris and Morris 1966; Kleiman et al. 1979; Schaller et al. 1985; White et al. 2002)
  • Urine and gland secretions
    • Both males and females mark territory with sticky secretions from anogenital glands (located in and near a panda’s anus and genitals) (Morris and Morris 1966; Kleiman et al. 1979; Schaller et al. 1985)
      • Leave marks on horizontal and vertical surfaces
    • Males also scent mark in a “headstand” position (raise hindquarters) to deposit scent higher on vertical surfaces (Kleiman and Collins 1972; Schaller et al. 1985; Swaisgood et al. 2000; White et al. 2002)
      • Thought to convey information about a male’s competitive ability, or behavioral intent to defend territory or mates (White et al. 2002)
      • Higher marks left by “better” competitors (White et al. 2002)
      • Females do not scent-mark in handstand position (Swaisgood et al. 2000)
  • Rubbing and rolling
    • Rub cheeks, neck and shoulders, and haunches on rocks and other firm surfaces (Kleiman et al. 1979)
    • Roll on ground, often rubbing their back, to adhere scents of other pandas onto their fur (Chorn and Hoffman 1978 citing Kleiman and Collins 1972) (Kleiman et al. 1979)
    • Rub sod, soil, ice, food, and grass into belly fur with front paws (Chorn and Hoffman 1978 citing Kleiman and Collins 1972) (Kleiman et al. 1979) 
  • Discriminating among chemical signals
    • Can detect age, sex, and reproductive condition of a panda who deposited their scent (Swaisgood et al. 2000; Swaisgood et al. 2002; White 2003; Swaisgood et al. 2004)
    • Able to distinguish individual identities of other pandas (Swaisgood et al. 1999; Swaisgood et al. 2004)
    • Can tell their relatives (kin) apart from unrelated individuals (Gilad et al. 2016)
      • Females spend more time investigating the scents of unrelated pandas (Gilad et al. 2016)
  • Functions and information encoded
    • Convey information about reproductive maturity, breeding status, and competitive status (Swaisgood et al. 2004)
    • Guard territory (males), guard mates (males), or rare denning sites (females)
    • Attend to certain portions of chemical signals more than others, depending on the season and individual motivations (White et al. 2004)
    • Prevent escalation of conflict or competition for resources (White et al. 2002)

Visual communication

  • Few noticeable body postures (Kleiman et al. 1979)
    • Emotional states and intentions subtle (Kleiman et al. 1979)
  • Visual signals
    • Often used in tandem with olfactory marking (see “Smell and scent marking”) (Schaller et al. 1985)
    • Behaviors
      • Strip tree bark (using teeth or claws) (Schaller et al. 1985)
      • Bite tree saplings (Schaller et al. 1985)
        • Remove a portion
        • Breaking trunk off completely
      • Leave claw marks (Schaller et al. 1985)
        • Sometimes faint
      • Paw ground surface, scraping away sod or snow (Schaller et al. 1985)
        • Observed on raised ground or at base of trees (Schaller et al. 1985)
  • Behavioral displays
    • Signal each other with body postures (Schaller et al. 1985)
      • Often accompanied by vocalizations
    • Play/non-aggression
      • Elicit other pandas to play by rolling and somersaulting (Wilson and Kleiman 1974)
      • Also see Play
    • Aggression
      • Stare at perceived opponent, suggesting a threat (Schaller et al. 1985)
      • Lower head and sometimes head-bob (Schaller et al. 1985)
      • See Aggression for description of other aggressive behaviors
    • Submission
      • Turn head away (Schaller et al. 1985)
      • Drop head, and cover eye patches and muzzle with paws (Schaller et al. 1985)
  • Coloration
    • Function of dark eye patches not clear, but might be used to visually communicate with other pandas and/or other animals (Schaller et al. 1985; Schaller 1994; Caro et al. 2017)
      • Emphasis on the eyes (as in, a stare–threat) may signal other pandas to keep away (Schaller 1994)
    • Head markings might also be used by pandas to recognize one another (Dungl et al. 2008; Caro et al. 2017)
      • More research needed

Vocal communication

  • Adult vocalizations
    • More than 10 distinctive vocal and non-vocal sounds identified for adult pandas (Schaller et al. 1985; Kleiman and Peters 1990)
      • Includes bleats, chirps, snorts, huffs, honks, moans, barks, squeals, growls, roars, etc. (Schaller et al. 1985; Kleiman and Peters 1990; Charlton et al. 2011)
        • Giant pandas are the only bears that do not hum (Peters et al. 2007)
      • Female and male sounds are similar (Schaller et al. 1985)
    • Breeding calls
      • Females give distinctive chirps when ready to mate (advertising fertility) (Kleiman and Peters 1990; Charlton et al. 2010)
      • Male bleats convey information about male hormone levels, such as androgens (Charlton et al. 2011)
        • May influence female mate choice and avoidance behavior by males
  • Cub vocalizations
    • Given at lower frequencies than adults’ courtship vocalizations (Owen et al. 2016a)
  • Impacts of noise pollution
    • Still being studied
    • Mother’s ability to hear cub’s lower-frequency calls may be hindered by noisy conditions, especially outside quieter old-growth forests (see Owen et al. 2016a)

Tactile communication

  • Most common during breeding period (Kleiman et al. 1979)
  • Biting, pawing, swatting, pouncing, pushing (Kleiman et al. 1979)

Agonistic Behavior & Defense

Aggressive behaviors

  • Swatting with forepaws (Schaller et al. 1985)
  • Lunging (Schaller et al. 1985)
  • Grappling (Schaller et al. 1985)
  • Biting (Schaller et al. 1985)

Territorial Behavior

Not territorial

  • Females
    • Do not patrol boundaries of the core area within their home range (Schaller et al. 1985)
  • Males
    • Not territorial (Schaller et al. 1985)
  • See “Home ranges,” above

Other Behaviors

Play

  • Common in young pandas (Chorn and Hoffman 1978)
    • Infrequently observed among pandas in wild populations, except between cubs and their mothers (Schaller et al. 1985)
  • Roll, tumble, somersault, and twist body (Wilson and Kleiman 1974; Chorn and Hoffman 1978; Kleiman et al. 1979)
  • Paw, swat, and pounce at playmate (Kleiman et al. 1979)
  • Slide down snowy slopes (Schaller et al. 1985)
  • In zoos, young pandas also observed rolling in dirt and bathing in water (Chorn and Hoffman 1978)

Locomotion

Walking

  • Agility
    • Can walk over rough terrain and through bamboo easily (Schaller et al. 1985)
  • Gait
    • Gait described as “rolling,” “ambling,” or “waddling” (Schaller et al. 1985)
      • Head held low (Schaller et al. 1985)
    • Move legs on same side of body (Davis 1964; Chorn and Hoffman 1978)
      • Similar gait to other bears (Chorn and Hoffman 1978)
    • Do not walk on hind legs but can stand upright (Chorn and Hoffman 1978)
  • Stride length
    • Longer stride than other bears (Chorn and Hoffman 1978)

Running

  • Rarely run, but will dash off if startled (Schaller et al. 1985)
    • Fast movement difficult in thick bamboo forest

Climbing

  • Infrequently climb trees as adults (Garshelis 2009b)
    • Spend less time in trees than other bears; forage on the ground, not in trees (Garshelis 2009b)
  • Functions/benefits
    • Climb trees to rest, escape danger, or for courtship (Garshelis 2009b)
  • Cubs and juveniles
    • Young pandas (between 4 and 14 months old) commonly stay in a tree while mother forages (Lü et al. 1994)
  • Adults
    • Climb trees to avoid hunting dogs (Morris and Morris 1966) (Chorn and Hoffman 1978)
    • Females in estrous may climb to avoid male suitors (Garshelis 2009b)

Panda at Rest

Giant Panda in a tree

Pandas are adept tree climbers when seeking a bit of sun, some peace and quiet, or safe refuge.

Image credit: © San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance. All rights reserved.

Page Citations

Catton (1990)
Charlton et al. (2009)
Charlton et al. (2010)
Garshelis (2004)
Hagey & MacDonald (2003)
Kleiman & Peters (1990)
Lü et al. (2008)
Mainka & Zhang (1994)
Nie et al. (2012)
Schaller et al. (1985)
Swaisgood et al. (2000)
White et al. (2002, 2003)
Zhang et al. (2014)

SDZWA Library Links