Skip to Main Content
San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance logo
San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance Library logo

African Elephants (Loxodonta africana and L. cyclotis) Fact Sheet: Behavior & Ecology

Update in Progress

Dear Readers,

This fact sheet, like an elephant, is aging gracefully. San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance is working to bring you an updated version of the African Elephants Fact Sheet with new science and conservation information. Thanks for your patience, as our tusks go to the ground and dig into this huge project. Please check back soon. SDZWA team members can email questions to library@sdzwa.org.

Looking for more elephant facts? Get a trunkful of SDZWA stories and news.

Activity Patterns

Daily patterns

  • Savanna elephants
    • Active day and night, but usually rest during hottest times of day (Guy 1976; Leggett 2010; Nowak 2018)
    • More active at night during dry seasons and in dry areas (Loarie, Aarde, et al. 2009), and where poachers or human disturbance occur (Von Gerhardt et al. 2014; Gaynor et al. 2018; Ihwagi et al. 2018)
  • Forest elephants
    • Early research suggests activity during the day and at night (Wrege et al. 2012; Turkalo et al. 2013; Kely et al. 2019; Reyna-Hurtado et al. 2023)
    • Some studies suggest feeding until about 14:00 and greater variety of behavior—excavating, bathing, fighting, sleeping—in the afternoon (Nowak 2018)
    • In one study, moved between forest and grasslands at dawn and dusk (Mills et al. 2018)
    • Avoid poachers and human activity during the day, similar to savanna elephant (Wrege et al. 2010; Beirne et al. 2021)

Feeding

  • Accounts for majority of wakeful activity (Beekman and Prins 1989)
  • Foraging in the morning thought to help elephants conserve energy and reduce heat stress (eg, Du Plessis et al. 2021)
  • Savanna elephant
    • Generally, at least half of active time spent feeding (eg, Wyatt and Eltringham 1974; Lindsay 2011; see literature reviewed by Sach et al. 2019)
      • May be up to 80%, when less food available (e.g., dry season, drought years)
  • Forest elephant
    • Thought to forage mainly during the day (Blake 2002; Turkalo and Barnes 2013)
  • Also see Diet & Feeding

Resting and sleeping

  • Savanna elephant
    • Generally, rest for short periods several times a day (eg, Wittemyer et al. 2017)
      • Sleep for relatively brief periods (typically about 1 to 2 hours), for a total of about 3 to 5 hours in each 24-hour period (Eltringham 1982; Estes 1993; Wittemyer et al. 2017)
    • Commonly rest/sleep late at night, between midnight and dawn, and during the early afternoon (hottest time of day) (eg, Wittemyer et al. 2017; Nowak 2018)
      • Desert populations may have distinct nighttime rest periods (Leggett 2010)
    • May sleep at night lying down, but usually rest standing up during the day (Eltringham 1982; Poole et al. 2013)
    • For groups, sleeping and waking often synchronized within the herd (Eltringham 1982)

Movements and Dispersal

Human impacts

  • Today, elephant ranges and movements mainly structured by human settlements and land use (including managed protected areas) (eg, Blake et al. 2008; Molina‐Vacas et al. 2020; Gobush et al. 2021; Leimgruber and Songer 2021; Wall et al. 2021)
    • Green forage, surface water, and other environmental factors influence elephant space use within range constraints exerted by humans (Young et al. 2009; Wall et al. 2021; Benitez et al. 2022)

Home range

  • Savanna elephants
    • Annual range size of roughly 100 to 300 km2, according to a continent-wide study by Wall et al. (2021)
      • Largest home ranges observed in desert-dwelling elephants (eg, Wall et al. 2013)
    • Males and females typically use distinct areas, except during breeding (see Poole et al. 2013)
    • Move to find water and high-quality forage (Bohrer et al. 2014; Nowak 2018)
    • Elephants constantly learn and recall detailed knowledge of landscapes (Polansky et al. 2015 and see Presotto et al. 2019)
  • Forest elephants
    • Annually, range over an area of less than 100 km2, up to 2,000 km2 (Turkalo and Barnes 2013; Beirne et al. 2021)
    • May move more during the wet season, while seeking out preferred fruits (Mills et al. 2018; Beirne et al. 2020)
    • Home ranges of females slightly smaller than males (Beirne et al. 2021; Wall et al. 2021)
    • More study needed, but at least some individuals appear to continually shift and explore their home range (Turkalo et al. 2013; Beirne et al. 2021)
      • Similar to savannah elephant (eg, Goldenberg et al. 2018)

Migration

  • Savanna elephant
    • Various movement patterns reported in different regions, depending on amount of food and water available (Gobush et al. 2022)
      • Resident, migratory, semi-migratory, or nomadic-like movements
      • Some herds show strong fidelity to their past wet or dry season range areas (eg, Burton-Roberts et al. 2022)
    • May move long distances each year in search of green vegetation and water (Bohrer et al. 2014; Tshipa et al. 2017; Nowak 2018)
      • Do not move far if food, water, and shade remain available across seasons (Nowak 2018)
      • Artificial (human-made) water holes expand elephants’ dry season range (see Loarie, Aarde, et al. 2009) and allow some elephants to remain resident year-round (Tshipa et al. 2017)
    • During drought, experienced matriarchs use their knowledge of water and food sources to lead herds across long distances (eg, Foley et al. 2008; Byrne and Bates 2011; also see McComb et al. 2001)
      • Similar leadership roles recently discovered among male elephants (see O’Connell-Rodwell et al. 2024)
    • May use environmental cues, such as rainfall, to time movements as the wet season begins (Kioko et al. 2020; Pandraud et al. 2022)
  • Forest elephants
    • Seasonal movements influenced by temperature and rainfall, despite living in wet forests (Beirne et al. 2020; Beirne et al. 2021)
    • Tend to use similar areas from year to year, though males may be more migratory and females more resident (Turkalo and Barnes 2013; Turkalo and Fay 1995; Turkalo et al. 2013; Molina‐Vacas et al. 2020; Beirne et al. 2021)

Dispersal

  • Savanna elephants
    • Males (Vidya and Sukumar 2005; Poole et al. 2013 citing Lee and Moss 1999)
      • Near puberty, leave birth family for extended periods to socialize with other families (Evans and Harris 2008; Lee, Poole, et al. 2011; Nowak 2018)
        • Later, disperse and leave birth family permanently
      • Full independence reached gradually (Lee and Moss 1999)
        • Process can take 5 months to as long as 8 years [Amboseli population] (Lee, Poole, et al. 2011)
        • On average, occurs at around 13 to 14 years old—but ages range from 7 to 19 years old [Amboseli, Samburu populations] (Lee, Poole, et al. 2011; Wittemyer et al. 2013)
    • Females
      • Continue living with birth family, though might disperse as a family group, singly, or in female pairs—eg, to recolonize unoccupied habitat areas (Ahlering et al. 2012)
  • Forest elephants
    • Both sexes gradually separate from birth family (Turkalo et al. 2018)
      • Female forest elephant behavior very different from female savannah elephant, where daughters usually remain with herds that include their mothers, sisters, and grandmothers (matriarchal groups) (Turkalo et al. 2018)
      • Possible social strategy to prevent competition between offspring and their parents (Turkalo et al. 2018; Goldenberg et al. 2021)
        • Daughters may disperse when ready to give birth to their first calf or when mother dedicates increased care to younger siblings (Turkalo et al. 2018; Goldenberg et al. 2021)
    • Age at independence
      • About 15 years old (median age for both males and females) (Turkalo et al. 2018)
        • But some individuals may separate from mother or conceive at younger ages (Turkalo 2013; Turkalo et al. 2017; Turkalo et al. 2018)
        • Reach full independence months or years after initial separation (Turkalo et al. 2017; Turkalo et al. 2018)

Territorial Behavior

Home ranges commonly overlap

  • Not territorial (both species) (Shoshani 1992; Poole et al. 2013; Nowak 2018)
  • Generally, home ranges overlap, especially those of family groups (Douglas-Hamilton 1972; Nowak 2018)
    • But groups may segregate by social rank or use home ranges with less overlap to avoid competing for resources (eg, during drought) (eg, Wittemyer et al. 2007; Blake and Maisels 2023)

Social Behavior

Social structure

  • Fission–fusion social structure in both species (eg, Moss and Poole 1983; Wittemyer et al. 2005; Archie et al. 2006; Wittemyer et al. 2009; Moss and Lee 2011b; Fishlock and Lee 2013; Head et al. 2013; Poole et al. 2013; Athira and Vidya 2021)
    • Members of a group change often, coming together to form larger groups and splitting apart
      • Fusing more common when food more abundant
      • Splitting more common when food is scarce
    • Dynamics influenced by many factors, including social bonds, season, home ranges, habitat, reproductive state, and genetic relatedness
    • Excellent memory for recognizing family members and distinguishing between familiar/unfamiliar individuals
  • Savanna elephants
    • Adults usually split into same-sex groups (Moss and Poole 1983; Kioko et al. 2020)
      • Adult males usually associate with females for only short periods
      • Also see “Social associations among males,” below
    • Herd behavior is highly gregarious (eg, Douglas-Hamilton 1972; Lee 1987; McComb et al. 2011; Nowak 2018)
      • Group members cooperate to find resources, defend the herd, and care for calves (females)
        • Also see Defense and Parental Care
    • Females form complex social bonds—among the most complex of any animal (eg, Moss and Poole 1983; Moss and Lee 2011b; Goldenberg et al. 2016)
      • Solitary females very rare (Nowak 2018)
    • Groups tend to consist of adult females—primarily female relatives and their offspring (e.g., Douglas-Hamilton 1972; Douglas-Hamilton 1972 citing Laws and Parker 1968; Archie et al. 2006; Moss and Lee 2011b; Charif et al. 2015)
      • Led by matriarch
      • Often multi-generational, where grandmothers, and their daughters and granddaughters live together
    • Usually about 10 to 25 individuals in a core group (family group) (Kingdon 1979; Young et al. 2009; McKnight 2015)
      • But have relationships with extended family, so groups may become several times larger—more than 50 individuals (Owens and Owens 2009; Moss and Lee 2011b; Poole et al. 2013)
      • Mixed groups (eg, breeding groups) may include 1 or more adult males (eg, McKnight 2015)
      • Where poaching occurs, a “group” may be reduced to a single female and her one offspring (Owens and Owens 2009; Moss and Lee 2011b)
    • Affiliations beyond extended family groups known as “clans” (Laws 1969; Moss and Poole 1983; Wittemyer et al. 2005; Archie et al. 2011; Poole et al. 2013; Charif et al. 2015; Nowak 2018)
      • For detailed information about core, bond, and clan groups, see Wittemyer et al. (2005) and review by Athira and Vidya (2021)
    • Large, temporary aggregations in open habitats can reach into the hundreds of individuals (Moss 1988; Owens and Owens 2009; Poole et al. 2013; McKnight 2015; Nowak 2018)
  • Forest elephants
    • Very different social organization than savanna elephant—more similar to Asian elephants, which also commonly live in forests (eg, White et al. 1993; Turkalo and Fay 1995; Turkalo et al. 2017; Goldenberg et al. 2021; Blake and Maisels 2023)
      • Difference may be due to patchy food or difficulty of traveling through dense forest in larger groups
      • Social role of matriarchs limited or absent outside of small family groups, which are comprised of mothers and their offspring
        • No extended matriarchal hierarchies, as in savanna elephants
    • Females typically observed in small groups of 2 to 5 individuals, but also commonly observed singly (White et al. 1993; Turkalo and Fay 1995; Turkalo et al. 2013; Nowak 2018; Reyna-Hurtado et al. 2023)
      • Groups commonly comprised of 1 or 2 related females and their dependent young (White et al. 1993; Turkalo and Fay 1995; Turkalo et al. 2013; Turkalo and Barnes 2013; Goldenberg et al. 2021; Reyna-Hurtado et al. 2023)
    • Core groups have stable associations over time (Goldenberg et al. 2021; Blake and Maisels 2023)
      • Uncommonly, 2 or 3 core groups may temporarily fuse (Head et al. 2013)
    • Sometimes aggregate in natural forest clearings and at mineral springs (Turkalo and Fay 1995; Fishlock and Lee 2013; Turkalo et al. 2013; Turkalo and Barnes 2013; Goldenberg et al. 2021)
      • Larger family units of up to 20 individuals observed at Dzanga clearing, Central African Republic (Turkalo and Barnes 2013)
    • Adult males mainly associate with females during breeding (Turkalo et al. 2013; Nowak 2018)

Social associations among males

  • Savanna elephants
    • Solitary males common (Chiyo et al. 2011; Nowak 2018)
    • Some males not very sociable (Poole 1987; Ahlering et al. 2012; Nowak 2018), some associate with unrelated males, and others still more highly bonded (O’Connell-Rodwell et al. 2024)
      • Males (not in musth) can have temporary, yet sometimes strong social ties (Goldenberg et al. 2014; Nowak 2018; O’Connell-Rodwell et al. 2024)
      • Other adult males maintain stable social relationships (in connected networks) over long time periods (Murphy et al. 2019; O’Connell-Rodwell et al. 2024)
    • Group sizes
      • McKnight (2015) reported groups of up to 18 males
    • Older males—or those more integrated or dominant in a group’s social network—strengthen associations among group members (Chiyo et al. 2011) and are more likely to lead group movements (Allen et al. 2020; O’Connell-Rodwell et al. 2024)
      • Younger males—those without in-depth spatial and ecological knowledge—thought to follow and gain ecological knowledge from experienced, adult males (Allen et al. 2020)
  • Forest elephants
    • Juvenile and subadult males associate temporarily for a day to several days (Turkalo and Barnes 2013)
    • Adult males usually live singly, outside of breeding periods (White et al. 1993; Turkalo and Fay 1995; Fishlock and Lee 2013; Head et al. 2013; Head et al. 2013; Turkalo and Barnes 2013; Nowak 2018)
      • Rarely form bachelor groups (of only 2 to 3 individuals)

Dominance hierarchies

  • Savanna elephants
    • Strong linear dominance hierarchies (Archie et al. 2006; Wittemyer and Getz 2007; de Silva et al. 2017)
    • Individual and group dominance structured by age (older = typically more dominant) (eg, Athira and Vidya 2021)
      • Older, larger females dominant to smaller, younger females (Archie et al. 2006; Nowak 2018)
      • Larger families with older matriarchs dominant to smaller family groups (Mutinda et al. 2011)
        • Have better access to preferred habitats and water, particularly during the dry season (Wittemyer et al. 2007)
    • Males in musth, especially those of larger body size, dominant to males not in musth (eg, Poole and Moss 1981; Hall-Martin 1987; Poole 1987; Poole 1989a)
      • Presence of adult males regulates behavior of younger males (eg, less defensive/aggressive and less fearful) (eg, Slotow et al. 2000; Allen et al. 2021)
    • Males form linear hierarchies when resources (eg, water) are limited (O’Connell-Rodwell et al. 2011)
  • Forest elephants
    • Males confront other males in forest clearings and at waterholes to establish/maintain their dominance status (Turkalo and Fay 1995; Turkalo and Barnes 2013; Nowak 2018)
    • Adult males more dominant than females and juvenile males (eg, at waterholes) (Turkalo and Fay 1995)
      • When present, tend to control access to preferred waterholes and minerals for long periods (Turkalo and Fay 1995; Wrege et al. 2012)

Orphaned calves—challenges

  • Calves unlikely to survive if orphaned before reaching 2 years old (Poole and Cobb 1989; Poole and Thomsen 1989; Moss and Lee 2011; Parker et al. 2021)
  • Survivors may or may not become fully integrated into a new social group (Goldenberg and Wittemyer 2018)
  • Weaker social bonds with adult females can result in orphans having less access to resources, such as food, water, or shade (Goldenberg and Wittemyer 2017; Goldenberg and Wittemyer 2018)
  • May not gain social or nuanced survival knowledge typically learned from members of their birth family (Goldenberg and Wittemyer 2017; McComb, Shannon, et al. 2011; Shannon et al. 2022)
  • Orphans may be subjected to more aggression from group members, compared to non-orphans, which likely causes more stress (Goldenberg and Wittemyer 2018)
    • However, some elephants may be able to adapt to prolonged stress (see Parker et al. 2022; Pokharel and Brown 2023)
  • Stunted growth observed among orphans of one Kenyan population (Parker and Wittemyer 2022)

Other Behaviors

Play behavior

  • Observed from infancy through adulthood, though declines over a lifetime (Lee and Moss 2014)
  • Elephants curious and quick to investigate (Poole and Granli 2011)
    • May use all parts of body—trunk, mouth, tusk, feet, etc.
  • Helps juveniles develop physical abilities, as well as social skills (eg, Evans and Harris 2008; Lee and Moss 2014)
    • Females: develop protector and leadership roles within families; play with younger calves
    • Males: engage with non-relatives; develop social relationships
  • Both gentle and “rough-and-tumble” play observed (eg, Evans and Harris 2008; Poole and Granli 2011; Lee and Moss 2014)
    • Wrestle, shove, and spar
    • Splash and spray water
    • Roll in mud
    • Mock charge and chase other elephants
    • Manipulate objects or lift/thrash vegetation
    • Give trumpet sounds (Poole 2011)
    • Young elephants chase mammals and birds (Poole et al. 2013)

 

Grief and consolation behaviors

  • Elephants investigate, approach, smell, and touch other dead elephants, including relatives and non-relatives (eg, Moss 1992; Payne 2003; Douglas-Hamilton et al. 2006; Poole and Granli 2011; Hawley et al. 2018; Goldenberg and Wittemyer 2020; Stephan et al. 2020)
    • Some other example responses include social behaviors, nudging or lifting behaviors, guarding the body, fear-related behaviors, picking up bones (Moss 1992; Poole 1996; Payne 2003; Douglas-Hamilton et al. 2006; Goldenberg and Wittemyer 2020; Stephan et al. 2020), attempting to feed an elephant that died, or covering the dead body with dirt or vegetation (Moss 1992; Poole and Granli 2011)
      • Behavior varies greatly from individual to individual
    • Lifting the body and vocalizations reported only when an elephant had recently died (Goldenberg and Wittemyer 2020)
  • Elephants may interact with dead individuals for extended periods, particularly using their highly developed sense of smell (Goldenberg and Wittemyer 2020)
  • Show interest in other kinds of dead animals (Poole et al. 2013)
    • Some individuals engage more with elephant remains vs. non-elephant remains (McComb et al. 2006; Goldenberg and Wittemyer 2020)

Communication //

Tactile

  • Family members communicate through touch
    • May rub with a foot or slap with the trunk, often while standing
    • Uses of the trunk
      • Trunk used in greeting
        • May be held out to an approaching elephant as a greeting
        • A lower-ranking animal will insert its trunk tip into the other’s mouth
      • Other uses of the trunk
        • An individual will caress another
        • Mother may guide her calf by gripping its tail with her trunk
        • Two individuals may intertwine trunks
        • Trunk used to check reproductive status

Vocalization

  • Click here for audio of the African Elephant. Provided by The Cornell Lab of Ornithology Macaulay Library.
  • Sound diversity
    • 4 different sounds are produced
      • Each is distinguished by gradations in pitch, duration, and volume
    • Rumbling
      • Frequencies variable; most are below human hearing
      • Functions
        • Important to sexual behavior; imparts distance information
          • Louder sounds may carry over several kilometers
        • A signal to more, or a warning
        • Softer rumbles may promote group cohesion
          • Quiet, though audible calls, heard during feeding events
        • Social greeting
          • Rumble or growl may be given as a greeting
        • Elevated, more bellowing rumbles given when in fear or pain
    • Trumpeting
      • Blowing through nostrils
      • Indicates excitement
    • Squealing
      • Juvenile distress call
    • Screaming
      • Adult distress call
      • Often in conjunction with trumpeting
      • Possibly meant to to intimidate another
  • Infrasonic communication
    • Discovered by Katherine Payne, based on observations of elephants raised under managed care made in 1984
  • Species specific characteristics
    • Savannah/bush elephants vocalize at 14-24 hertz
    • Forest elephants vocalize around 5 hertz

Olfaction/Scent Marking

  • Scent glands
    • Temporal gland secretions "musky"
      • Secretions is under autonomic control
      • Produced when an individual is excited or anxious

Locomotion

Walking

  • Adults generally walk (McKay 1973)
    • Only run when fleeing danger or charging
    • Move legs on same side of body, even at higher speeds (Hutchinson et al. 2003; Hutchinson et al. 2006)
      • Left hind, left front; right hind, right front (Hutchinson et al. 2006)
  • Basic gait
    • 3 feet on the ground most of the time
    • At least 1 foot always on ground
    • Body has a rolling motion and head bobs during fast walk
  • Speed
    • Slow walk
      • About 0.5 kph (0.3 mph) or slower (Ngene et al. 2010; Chamaillé‐Jammes et al. 2014; Troup et al. 2020)
        • When eating, resting, socializing in one location (Ngene et al. 2010)
      • 2 to 3 kph (1 to 2 mph) (Hutchinson 2006)
        • When shifting between resource patches
    • Faster walk
      • 6 to 8 kph (4 to 5 mph) (Estes 2012)

“Running”

  • Unusual hybrid gait for moving fast (Hutchinson et al. 2003 in Asian elephant)
    • Legs support bouncing motion, and joints bend more as speed increases (Hutchinson et al. 2003; Hutchinson et al. 2006)
  • Primarily increase speed by taking more strides (Hutchinson et al. 2006; Estes 2012)
  • Maximum speed
    • Up to about 26 kph (16 mph) (Hutchinson et al. 2003; Ngene et al. 2010; Estes 2012)

Jumping

  • Cannot jump (Estes 2012)
  • Have difficulty crossing some ditches/trenches
    • Generally unable to cross gaps wider than about 2 m (7 ft)

Other postures

  • Able to kneel, squat, roll, and sit on haunches (eg, when getting up and lying down) (Estes 2012; Poole et al. 2013)
  • Can balance back legs to reach browse up high (Estes 2012)

Interspecies Interactions //

Herbivore competitors

  • Coexist with 3 other large mammal species (Eltringham,1982)
    • Buffalo (Syncerus), bush pig (Potamochoerus), bushbuck (Tragelaphus scriptus)
  • Elephants typically ignore other herbivores
    • Such as antelopes, warthogs and zebras

Carnivore predators

  • Elephants seek out lions in their vicinity to chase them away

Elephants as "keystone" species

  • Elephants impact on animals and plants
    • Elephants may stop the progression of grassland to forest or shrub lands
      • Grasses support grazing ungulates which are in turn food for carnivores
    • Elephants maintain trails used by all species
    • Elephants keep down vegetation around water holes
      • Process creates safe drinking spots for all species

Elephant Mud Bath

an African elephant calf in mud

Elephant calf playing in the mud.

Image credit: © San Diego Zoo Wildlife Alliance. All rights reserved.

Page Citations

Caloi (1992)
Estes (1991)
Estes (1993)
Eltringham (1982)
Moss (2000)

SDZWA Library Links